Sunday, 30 June 2013

Did our brains evolve to foolishly follow celebrities? - BBC News - Viewpoint

BBC News - Viewpoint: Did our brains evolve to foolishly follow celebrities?


Composite of celebrities: Victoria and David Beckham; Paris Hilton; Angelina Jolie and Brad Pitt; Kim Kardashian; Katie Price

Our obsession with celebrity culture is a result of our poorly adapted brains, argues social anthropologist Jamie Tehrani.
I love a good quote.

One of my all-time favourite quotes comes from Mark Twain, who once wrote to his friend "I am sorry for the length of this letter, but I did not have the time to write a short one".

It's an apology I have often repeated in bloated ramblings to my friends and colleagues, and it's a wonderfully wry, pithy insight. Typical Twain, you might say.

Except that it's not. Because, as the person who recently pulled me up for using it told me, the true author of the quote is in fact a less well-known French thinker, Blaise Pascal, who coined it in a letter to a colleague in 1657. I looked it up and they were absolutely right.

And it turns out not to be the only quote I've been abusing.

I'm sure most of you are familiar with Einstein's brilliant refrain: "The definition of insanity is doing the same thing over and over again and expecting a different result." It's probably the most famous thing he said, apart from "E=mc2".

Only, there's no record of him every having uttered these words. The first time they appeared in print was in 1981, in a Narcotics Anonymous pamphlet, some 25 years after the great man died.
There are many, many similar examples.

Winston Churchill, Benjamin Franklin and Martin Luther King have probably said less than half the things you've heard them quoted on. Because quotes are just so much more quotable when they come from individuals who are famed for their wit and wisdom.

It's OK. Because misattributing quotes exemplifies our tendency to give too much credit to celebrities.
Fame is a powerful cultural magnet. As a hyper-social species, we acquire the bulk of our knowledge, ideas and skills by copying from others, rather than through individual trial-and-error. However, we pay far more attention to the habits and behaviours demonstrated by famous people than those demonstrated by ordinary members of our community.

Albert Einstein and Mark Twain  
Good quote, wrong person: Albert Einstein and Mark Twain
 
It follows that things are much more likely to catch on if they are associated with someone who is well known for one reason or another - even if the association is erroneous, as in the case of those Twain and Einstein misquotations. This raises the question of whether what is said is as important as who said it.
Another example of the way in which celebrities act as cultural magnets is that we frequently copy traits that have little, if anything, to do with what made them successful in the first place - like the clothes they wear, their hairstyles, or how they talk.
That's basically the reason that companies sponsor stars to use their products. Celebrities are always on the TV and in the media, so of course getting them to wear your brand of jeans or wristwatch is a great way to give them exposure.

But it's not just about getting your products in the public eye. You wouldn't know from images on TV or in a newspaper or on a computer screen what kind of underpants David Beckham wears, what coffee George Clooney drinks, or what perfume Beyonce smells of.

Stacks of underwear featuring David Beckham on the box

Companies get celebrities to advertise these kinds of products because they know that our perceptions of value are actively influenced by fame. Celebrity endorsements not only make products more visible, they make them more desirable.

So why is this? Celebrity culture is often portrayed as something relatively new, a product of a media-saturated but socially atomised society.

Although I agree that the form of celebrity culture has no doubt been shaped by the modern world, it is rooted in much more basic human instincts, which have played a key role in our acquisition of culture and have been crucial to the evolutionary success of our species.

We might focus on the anthropology of prestige. Prestige is a form of social status that is based on the respect and admiration of members of one's community. It is particularly interesting for anthropologists because it seems to be a unique characteristic of our species, and something that is universal to all human cultures.

A silverback gorilla  
 Social hierarchies in other primates are typically based on dominance
 
In other primates, social hierarchies are typically based on dominance, which is different from prestige because it implies fear, and the threat of violence.

Individuals defer to more dominant animals because if they fail to let them have what they want then it would be perceived as a challenge to their status, which they will defend by force. Many types of hierarchy in human society are similarly characterised by dominance.

However, unlike other primates, we also differentiate social status in terms of prestige. In contrast to dominance, prestige is given voluntarily. It is freely conferred to individuals in recognition of their achievements in a particular field, and is not backed up by force.

How did such systems arise? The most convincing theory suggests that prestige evolved as part of a package of psychological adaptations for cultural learning. It allowed our ancestors to recognise and reward individuals with superior skills and knowledge, and learn from them.

How prestige may have evolved
  • One theory suggests that prestige evolved as a part of a package of psychological adaptations for cultural learning
  • Rather than establishing important social status only through physical force and dominance - as in other primates - prestige allows different attributes to be rewarded
  • Individuals with superior skills and knowledge as gatherers, or in medicine, could therefore be equally as highly regarded as the fiercest warriors
  • By attaching significant prestige to these people, and learning or benefitting from their skills, both individuals' and the group's chances of survival were boosted
This allowed new discoveries and techniques - for instance, how to exploit the medicinal properties of plants or optimise the design of hunting weapons - to spread across the whole population, and enabled each successive generation to build on and improve the knowledge of their predecessors.
Although the bias for preferentially imitating prestigious individuals has generally helped promote the spread of adaptive behaviours, anthropologists have suggested that it can make us susceptible to copying traits that are of no use in themselves, or which may even be harmful.

The reason for this is that prestige-biased learning is a very general strategy that is targeted at successful role models, rather than specific traits. This is precisely what makes it such a powerful and flexible tool - because the traits that make someone successful will vary significantly in different environments, so it makes sense to copy whoever happens to be doing best at a particular time and place.

However, because this strategy is somewhat indiscriminate, it can lead to people adopting all kinds of behaviours exhibited by a role model, including ones that have nothing to do with their success.
For example, men might observe a successful hunter perform some kind of incantation at the same time as he re-touches his arrowheads, and adopt both the ritual as well as his knapping techniques as a single package when they prepare their own tools.

This tendency, I believe, explains our interest in what sports stars and singers wear, what car they drive, and where they go shopping.

Beyonce standing next to a bottle of perfume she is endorsing

In the past any useless traits we acquired as a result of prestige-biased learning were offset by the benefits of picking up useful skills. So, in the long-run, it was an effective, adaptive strategy.
However, I am far from convinced that our attraction to prestige continues to promote superior cultural knowledge and skill.

The modern world is very different from the one in which our brains evolved, and I believe that the originally adaptive bias for imitating successful people has today morphed into an unhealthy obsession with celebrities, who we give far more attention to than they deserve.

Let me illustrate the point by way of an analogy to diet. We have an evolved preference for sweet-tasting and fatty foods because they motivated our ancestors to seek out ripe fruits and meat, which are rich in essential nutrients. But in today's world of mass-produced confectionery and intensive agriculture, these previously adaptive tastes have led to a massive obesity epidemic and all the health problems it's associated with.

Similarly, we can think of the mass-media as junk food for the mind. Quick. Convenient. But not exactly nutritious. We gorge ourselves on images of wealth and success because they appeal to our appetite for prestige. But are celebrities actually good role models?

A hunter gatherer aims his bow and arrow
 Ancestral societies attached prestige to qualities such as hunting proficiency
 
In posing that question, I am not referring to well-publicised instances of bad behaviour by over-sexed footballers or drunken soap stars. I am talking on a more general level about the very purpose of celebrity itself.

In ancestral societies, the blueprint for a good role model was relatively well-defined - a good hunter, or gatherer, or parent, maybe a ritual expert.

But in our society, with its complex class system, division of labour and melting pot of cultures, the criteria for success are far more varied and opaque. Many celebrities have accomplished their success in fields like sport and music, which most of us have little hope of emulating.

What is evolutionary psychology?
  • Inspired by work of Charles Darwin and applies his theory of natural selection to the human mind and how it evolved to benefit the individual
  • From this point of view, complex aspects of human behaviour and experience - including language, memory, and consciousness - all evolved because of their adaptive fitness
  • In one way or another, these features promoted survival of the human species
  • Humans' large brains, for instance, are hypothesised to have evolved in response to our complex social organisation
  • Individuals who were better at manipulating others to meet their own needs were more likely to survive, and therefore bigger brains evolved out of natural selection
Read more at BBC Science: Body and Mind
But we still imitate what we can because our brains are programmed to associate prestige with adaptive behaviour. And because fame is the primary cue of prestige, the more attention celebrities get, the more they attract.
It's not surprising then, that fame has become an end in itself. Because in the modern world, it does not really matter what you are famous for.

Indeed, while celebrities today get more attention and prestige than at any other point in human history, we are frequently being told not to hold them up as role models.

But - seen from the evolutionary, anthropological perspective that I have sketched out - you may ask, what are celebrities for if they are not to be role models?

Why give them the benefits of our prestige, if it is not reciprocated with anything that might be of use to us?

In pondering those questions, we would do well to reflect on the words of Samuel Johnson: "To get a name is one of the few things that cannot be bought. It is the free gift of mankind, which must be deserved before it will be granted."

At least, I think that was Samuel Johnson.


About the author

Jamie Tehrani

Jamie Tehrani is a social anthropologist at Durham University, focusing particularly on the areas of cultural evolution and diffusion.

Hear more from him on Radio 4's Four Thought on Wednesday 26 June at 20:45 BST or download the Four Thought podcast

Saturday, 15 June 2013

A Psychosis Caused By Our Immune System | Psychology Today

A Psychosis Caused By Our Immune System | Psychology Today

The hunt for evolutionary solutions to contemporary mental health problems.


10 percent of schizophrenia cases found to be autoimmune-related in recent study

brain
Over the past several years, it has become clear that some psychopathology is caused by issues with the immune system, particularly when the immune system attacks our own body, called autoimmune disease. For example, women with post-partum psychosis are more likely than controls to have anti-thyroid antibodies. And folks with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder are more likely to have strange anti-wheat protein antibodies than controls. In the recent, very large CATIE trial,23.% of those with schizophrenia had IgA anti-AGA antibiodies (anti-gliadin) compared to 3.1% of a comparison group, and 5.4% had high levels of tTG antibodies compared to 0.8% of the comparison group. These antibodies are normally tested looking for celiac disease.

No one is sure what these immune reactions mean. But it would be interesting to see how immune modulators might affect psychosis in a clinical trial. In evolutionary medicine, immune and inflammatory modulators could include a dietary intervention, probiotics, or even helminth therapies. All of these could theoretically affect how our immune system reacts to both foreign and domestic (meaning our own) proteins and cell markers. To my knowledge, none of these have been applied to schizophrenia or post-partum psychosis in a clinical trial of any kind.

Earlier this year, a paper came out in the renamed Archives of General Psychiatry (Now JAMA Psychiatry) linking schizophrenia to a set of autoantibodies. The findings in this paper lend more credence to the idea that a subset of schizophrenia may be caused by an immune attack on the brain.  Blood from a group of unmedicated, hospitalized schizophrenics was compared to blood from people admitted with major depressive disorder, borderline personality disorder, and healthy controls.

9.9% of the actuely ill schizophrenics were found to have anti-NMDA receptor antibodies, compared with 2.8% of those with major depressive disorder, 0.4% of controls, and 0 of those with borderline personality disorder. The NMDA receptor (glutamate is the key neurotransmitter at this receptor) is known to be associated with psychotic symptoms. PCP and ketamine are NMDA receptor antagonists that rather famously cause agitation and psychosis.
antibody


Now there is already an illness of anti-NMDA receptors called "NMDA-R encephalitis." At last week's American Psychiatric Association annual meeting, a number of the poster presentations reviewed cases of NMDA-R encephalitis masking as schizophrenia. It typically affects young women with a rare type of ovarian tumor called a teratoma, and presents with psychosis, agitation, memory problems, and seizures. It tends to progress to problems with the autonomic nervous system (which can control breathing, temperature and blood pressure regulation) and cause a catatonic state. It is treated, like many life-threatening autoimmune conditions, with high dose steroids and plasmaphoresis (or plasma exchange, which can clear the blood of the offending autoantibodies).The autoantibodies in the cases of NMDA-R encephalitis are to a different specific protein subunit of the receptor and tend to be in much higher concentrations than the folks with autoantibodies who had acute schizophrenia, so it is not exactly the same disease.  In this trial, however, two of the patients originally diagnosed with schizophrenia were re-diagnosed as NMDA-R encephalitis due to the type of antibodies they had. They also had some intriguing physical symptoms and CNS and blood  inflammatory markers that aren't typically found in schizophrenia.

But it is fascinating and needs to be studied in more populations at greater length. Is there a time coming when 10% of our first break psychosis patients might be getting plasma exchange and steroids? Would they be maintained on autoimmune dietary protocols (if effective for blood titres of antibodies) and relatively benign chronic immune modulators (again, just hypothesizing in an exciting sort of way) such as pig whipworm or killed M vaccae?

As always, more questions than answers, but getting one step closer to the bottom of the pathology of mental illness and brain diseases is always interesting and always gives us hope for new and better therapeutic and preventative options. And what about the healthy control and the patients with major depressive disorder who had anti-NMDA-R antibodies? Are they more likely to have problems with psychosis or psychopathology? We will have to wait and see.
Images from wikimedia commons.
Copyright Emily Deans, M.D.

Friday, 14 June 2013

Studies in shame, fear & human connection - TED: Brene Brown




play full screen:  on you tube
Uploaded on 3 Jan 2011
 


http://www.ted.com

Brene Brown studies human connection -- our ability to empathize, belong, love. In a poignant, funny talk at TEDxHouston, she shares a deep insight from her research, one that sent her on a personal quest to know herself as well as to understand humanity. A talk to share.

TEDTalks is a daily video podcast of the best talks and performances from the TED Conference, where the world's leading thinkers and doers give the talk of their lives in 18 minutes. Featured speakers have included Al Gore on climate change, Philippe Starck on design, Jill Bolte Taylor on observing her own stroke, Nicholas Negroponte on One Laptop per Child, Jane Goodall on chimpanzees, Bill Gates on malaria and mosquitoes, Pattie Maes on the "Sixth Sense" wearable tech, and "Lost" producer JJ Abrams on the allure of mystery.

TED stands for Technology, Entertainment, Design, and TEDTalks cover these topics as well as science, business, development and the arts. Closed captions and translated subtitles in a variety of languages are now available on TED.com, at http://www.ted.com/translate. Watch a highlight reel of the Top 10 TEDTalks at http://www.ted.com/index.php/talks/top10


Tuesday, 4 June 2013

"Up/Down" Bipolar Disorder Documentary FULL (2011)



"Up/Down" Bipolar Disorder Documentary FULL MOVIE (2011)

Published on 1 Jan 2013
http://www.facebook.com/updownmovie
http://www.imdb.com/title/tt1821695/

There are approximately 5.7 million people in the United States with bipolar disorder. In an attempt to eliminate the mystery and misinformation surrounding the illness, many throughout the country diagnosed with this condition were interviewed extensively. They diligently explain the struggle to balance themselves between floating to a state of euphoria and sinking to a devastating depression.

In short, "Up/Down" is a personal analysis of bipolar disorder from those living with it. Looking for something with a more scientific or clinical angle? Try a textbook.

"Bipolar disorder is just one of many mental illnesses that is still highly stigmatized in our culture today, and 'Up/Down' could certainly be instrumental in changing that fact."
-Microfilmmaker Magazine, Issue 65

If you would like to purchase a copy of the film and support independent filmmakers (because not all of us operate on trust funds), here are the following methods:

https://www.createspace.com/340597
http://www.amazon.com/Up-Down/dp/B006...
http://cn.youreeeka.com/main/watch/942

Star Award, 2011 Indie Gathering
Merit Award, 2011 Accolade Competition
Art Exhibition Award, 2011 Love Unlimited Film Festival
Nominated for Best Feature Documentary, 2011 Los Angeles Reel Film Festival

Thank you for taking the time to watch the film!

Copyright Arpi-Revo Productions 2011

http://www.arpi-revo.com

Stephen Fry - The Secret Life Of the Manic-Depressive



Saturday, 1 June 2013

Misophonia Online | The HUB

Misophonia Online | The HUB

What is Misophonia?

Do you find yourself gnashing your teeth, clenching your hands, wanting – needing – to scream with rage for what seems like no reason, except that someone nearby is clicking a pen, eating a carrot, using a lot of “s” words in a conversation? Have you thought something’s “wrong” with you because you feel such strong emotional responses to sounds others don’t even seem to hear? Guess what? You’re not alone!
Misophonia
Welcome to Misophonia Online – a website developed & maintained by volunteers to promote awareness of a condition in which the person suffers from an extreme intolerance to certain sounds – sounds that are considered normal everyday sounds.

The term Misophonia was developed by an American neuroscientists Pawel Jastreboff and Margaret Jastreboff, during this period Dr. Marsha Johnson (Aud) labeled this condition “Selective Sound Sensitivity Syndrome” to address the sub class of indivuals that did not hate all sound but only particular sounds. She  started a Yahoo support group which has 3,500+ members, and membership continues to grow. You may find the support group here. The condition has been called many things – Soft Sound Sensitivity Syndrome, SSSS, SSS, 4S, Hyperacusis, Phonophobia, Misophonia - for those who suffer, the name is not important. Far more important is the knowledge that those who suffer are not alone – that an entire support network does exist and that research for a cure is being conducted. Here, we will use the name “misophonia” as it literally means ‘the hatred of sound’.

On this website, you will find resources to help you learn about the condition, coping mechanisms and tips to help you reduce your symptoms. We have a forum with public and private support sections, as well as separate sections for friends and family members. We even have a self-test here so you may possibly further define your symptoms.

For more information about this condition and the stimuli that can trigger reactions for one with misophonia, please see our Symptoms & Triggers page.


Misophonia - Wikipedia

Misophonia - Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

also: http://misophonia.com/

Misophonia, literally “hatred of sound”[1], is a form of decreased sound tolerance. It is believed[2] to be a neurological disorder characterized by negative experiences resulting only from specific sounds, whether loud or soft.[3] The term was coined by American neuroscientists Pawel Jastreboff and Margaret Jastreboff.[4] The term is often used interchangeably with the term selective sound sensitivity.[5] Misophonia has not been classified as a discrete disorder in DSM-IV-TR or ICD-10.
Unlike hyperacusis, misophonia is specific for certain sounds. Little is known about the anatomical location of the physiological abnormality that causes such symptoms but it is most likely high central nervous system structures.[6] It has been speculated that the anatomical location may be more central than that involved in hyperacusis.[7]

Contents

Symptoms

People who have misophonia are most commonly annoyed, or even enraged, by such ordinary sounds as other people clipping their nails, brushing teeth, eating, breathing, sniffing, talking, sneezing, yawning, walking, chewing gum, laughing, snoring, whistling or coughing; certain consonants; or repetitive sounds.[8] Some are also affected by visual stimuli, such as repetitive foot or body movements, fidgeting or any movement they might observe out of the corner of their eyes. Intense anxiety and avoidant behavior may develop, which can lead to decreased socialization. Some people may feel the compulsion to mimic what they hear or see.[9]

Prevalence and comorbidity

The prevalence of misophonia is currently unknown but groups of people identifying with the condition suggest it is more common than previously recognized.[9] Among patients with tinnitus, which is prevalent in 4-5% of the general population,[10] some surveys report prevalence as high as 60%[9] while prevalence in a 2010 study was measured at 10%.[11]
A Dutch study published in 2013[12] with a sample of 42 patients with misophonia found a low incidence for most psychiatric disorders, the exception being Obsessive-Compulsive Personality Disorder (52.4%).

See also